Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Over the Right Anterior Temporal Lobe Does Not Modulate False Recognition

  1. Alonso, María Angeles 1
  2. Díez-Álamo, Antonio M. 2
  3. Gómez-Ariza, Carlos J. 3
  4. Díez, Emiliano 23
  5. Fernandez, Angel 23
  1. 1 Universidad de La Laguna
    info

    Universidad de La Laguna

    San Cristobal de La Laguna, España

    ROR https://ror.org/01r9z8p25

  2. 2 Universidad de Salamanca
    info

    Universidad de Salamanca

    Salamanca, España

    ROR https://ror.org/02f40zc51

  3. 3 Universidad de Jaén
    info

    Universidad de Jaén

    Jaén, España

    ROR https://ror.org/0122p5f64

Revue:
Frontiers in Psychology

ISSN: 1664-1078

Année de publication: 2021

Volumen: 12

Type: Article

DOI: 10.3389/FPSYG.2021.718118 GOOGLE SCHOLAR

D'autres publications dans: Frontiers in Psychology

Résumé

Non-invasive transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the left anterior temporal lobe (ATL) has been shown to cause a reduction in the rate of false memories with semantically related words. Such a reduction seems to be specific to false memories induced by the study of associative lists, but is not observed when the studied lists are categorical in nature. These findings are interpreted as evidence that the left ATL functions as an integration hub that is crucial for the binding of semantic information into coherent representations of concepts. In order to investigate whether the right ATL might also contribute to semantic integration in the processing of verbal associative material, a follow-up tDCS study was conducted with the stimulation at study lateralized on the right ATL. A sample of 75 undergraduate students participated in an experiment in which they studied 8 associative lists and 8 categorical lists. One third of the participants studied all their word lists under anodal stimulation, another third studied under cathodal stimulation and the other third under sham stimulation. Results showed that stimulation of the right ATL by tDCS does not modulate false recognition for either association-related critical words or category-related critical words. These results provide preliminary support to views positing asymmetric connectivity between the anterior temporal lobes and the semantic representational network, and provide evidence for understanding bilateral brain dynamics and the nature of semantically induced memory distortions.

Références bibliographiques

  • Acharya, (2016), J. Clin. Neurophysiol., 33, pp. 312, 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000317
  • Antal, (2007), Eur. J. Neurosci., 26, pp. 2687, 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05896.x
  • Antonenko, (2018), Neurobiol. Aging, 61, pp. 245, 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.09.017
  • Barrick, (2007), Cerebral Cortex, 17, pp. 591, 10.1093/cercor/bhk004
  • Berryhill, (2014), Front. Psychol., 5, pp. 800, 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00800
  • Bestmann, (2015), Trends Cogn. Sci., 19, pp. 13, 10.1016/j.tics.2014.10.003
  • Bindman, (1964), J. Physiol., 172, pp. 369, 10.1113/jphysiol.1964.sp007425
  • Binney, (2015), Neuropsychologia, 76, pp. 170, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.009
  • Bjekić, (2019), Neurobiol. Learn. Mem., 157, pp. 114, 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.12.007
  • Boggio, (2009), PLoS ONE, 4, pp. e4959, 10.1371/journal.pone.0004959
  • Bonner, (2013), J. Neurosci., 33, pp. 4213, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0041-13.2013
  • Brainerd, (2005), The Science of False Memory, 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154054.001.0001
  • Brückner, (2017), Neuroscience, 343, pp. 269, 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.015
  • Brunoni, (2011), Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol., 14, pp. 1133, 10.1017/S1461145710001690
  • Cambiaghi, (2010), Eur. J. Neurosci., 31, pp. 704, 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07092.x
  • Cerruti, (2009), J. Cogn. Neurosci., 21, pp. 1980, 10.1162/jocn.2008.21143
  • Chadwick, (2016), Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 113, pp. 10180, 10.1073/pnas.1610686113
  • Chiou, (2018), Cortex, 103, pp. 100, 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.02.018
  • Coane, (2021), Front. Psychol., 12, pp. 668550, 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.668550
  • Coane, (2016), Mem. Cognit., 44, pp. 37, 10.3758/s13421-015-0543-1
  • Dalton, (2016), Hum. Brain Mapp., 37, pp. 933, 10.1002/hbm.23077
  • de Boysson, (2011), Brain Cogn., 75, pp. 111, 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.011
  • Deese, (1959), J. Exp. Psychol., 58, pp. 17, 10.1037/h0046671
  • Díez, (2017), Cortex, 93, pp. 133, 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.004
  • Donaldson, (1996), Mem. Cognit., 24, pp. 523, 10.3758/BF03200940
  • Faul, (2007), Behav. Res. Methods, 39, pp. 175, 10.3758/BF03193146
  • FernandezA. DíezE. AlonsoM. A. Normas de Asociación libre en castellano de la Universidad de Salamanca2014
  • Fernandez, (2004), Behav. Res. Methods Instr. Comput., 36, pp. 577, 10.3758/BF03195604
  • Fertonani, (2017), Neuroscientist, 23, pp. 109, 10.1177/1073858416631966
  • Filmer, (2014), Trends Neurosci., 37, pp. 742, 10.1016/j.tins.2014.08.003
  • Fiori, (2011), J. Cogn. Neurosci., 23, pp. 2309, 10.1162/jocn.2010.21579
  • Fiori, (2017), Behav. Brain Res., 321, pp. 170, 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.12.044
  • Flöel, (2012), Neurobiol. Aging, 33, pp. 1682, 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.05.007
  • Friedrich, (2018), Sci. Rep., 30, pp. 17486, 10.1038/s41598-018-35879-2
  • Gainotti, (2011), Neuropsychologia, 49, pp. 2273, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.027
  • Gainotti, (2012), Cortex, 48, pp. 521, 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.019
  • Gainotti, (2020), Cogn. Neuropsychol., 37, pp. 466, 10.1080/02643294.2020.1739011
  • Gallate, (2009), Neurosci. Lett., 449, pp. 151, 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.11.021
  • Galli, (2019), Brain Stimul., 12, pp. 231, 10.1016/j.brs.2018.11.008
  • Gallo, (2006), Associative Illusions of Memory
  • Gallo, (2010), Mem. Cognit., 38, pp. 833, 10.3758/MC.38.7.833
  • Gómez-Ariza, (2017), Front. Neurosci., 11, pp. 282, 10.3389/fnins.2017.00282
  • Guillory, (2010), Psychon. Bull. Rev., 17, pp. 73, 10.3758/PBR.17.1.73
  • Hoffman, (2018), Psychol. Rev., 125, pp. 293, 10.1037/rev0000094
  • King, (2020), Neuroimage, 223, pp. 117323, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117323
  • Köhler, (2020), Neuropsychologia, 138, pp. 107339, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107339
  • Kurkela, (2016), Neuropsychologia, 81, pp. 149, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.006
  • Lambon Ralph, (2010), Brain, 133, pp. 3243, 10.1093/brain/awq264
  • Lambon Ralph, (2014), Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B, 369, pp. 20120392, 10.1098/rstb.2012.0392
  • Lambon Ralph, (2017), Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 18, pp. 42, 10.1038/nrn.2016.150
  • Lambon Ralph, (2008), Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1124, pp. 61, 10.1196/annals.1440.006
  • Leach, (2019), J. Gerontol. Series B, 74, pp. 1163, 10.1093/geronb/gby003
  • Marful, (2015), Behav. Res. Methods, 47, pp. 902, 10.3758/s13428-014-0513-8
  • Matzen, (2015), Brain Res., 1624, pp. 286, 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.036
  • McCabe, (2004), Psychon. Bull. Rev., 11, pp. 1074, 10.3758/BF03196739
  • Nitsche, (2000), J. Physiol., 527, pp. 633, 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x
  • Nitsche, (2001), Neurology, 57, pp. 1899, 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899
  • Oldfield, (1971), Neuropsychologia, 9, pp. 97, 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
  • Patterson, (2016), Neurobiology of Language, pp. 765, 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00061-4
  • Patterson, (2007), Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 8, pp. 976, 10.1038/nrn2277
  • Peretz, (2013), Cortex, 49, pp. 1056, 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.03.015
  • Pergolizzi, (2015), Neuropsychologia, 66, pp. 88, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.012
  • Pergolizzi, (2016), Brain Cogn., 108, pp. 56, 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.06.009
  • Pisoni, (2015), PLoS ONE, 10, pp. e0123085, 10.1371/journal.pone.0123085
  • Price, (2016), J. Neurosci., 36, pp. 3829, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3120-15.2016
  • E-prime 3.02016
  • Vienna, AustriaR Foundation for Statistical ComputingR: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing2020
  • Rajaram, (1993), Mem. Cognit., 21, pp. 89, 10.3758/BF03211168
  • Rice, (2018), Cerebral Cortex, 28, pp. 1487, 10.1093/cercor/bhx362
  • Rice, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., 1359, pp. 84, 10.1111/nyas.12951
  • Rice, Cerebral Cortex, 25, pp. 4374, 10.1093/cercor/bhv024
  • Roediger, (2016), Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgment and Memory, pp. 390
  • Roediger, (1995), J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 21, pp. 803, 10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803
  • Roediger, (2001), The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Robert G. Crowder, pp. 95, 10.1037/10394-006
  • Salvi, (2020), Sci. Rep., 10, pp. 946, 10.1038/s41598-020-57724-1
  • Simons, (2005), Neuropsychology, 19, pp. 353, 10.1037/0894-4105.19.3.353
  • Smirni, (2015), PLoS ONE, 10, pp. e0144838, 10.1371/journal.pone.0144838
  • Snowden, (2004), Brain, 127, pp. 860, 10.1093/brain/awh099
  • Stagg, (2011), Neuropsychologia, 49, pp. 800, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.009
  • Stanislaw, (1999), Behav. Res. Methods Instr. Comput., 31, pp. 137, 10.3758/BF03207704
  • Tanaka, (2009), Exp. Brain Res., 196, pp. 459, 10.1007/s00221-009-1863-9
  • Tanaka, (2020), Front. Neurosci., 14, pp. 495, 10.3389/fnins.2020.00495
  • Thapar, (2001), Mem. Cognit., 29, pp. 424, 10.3758/BF03196393
  • jamovi (Version 1.6)2021
  • Tulving, (1985), Can. Psychol., 26, pp. 1, 10.1037/h0080017
  • Tussing, (1997), Psychon. Bull. Rev., 4, pp. 572, 10.3758/BF03214351
  • Wong, (2012), Brain Res., 1449, pp. 94, 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.017
  • Woollams, (2018), Cortex, 107, pp. 64, 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.11.014
  • (2013), J. Am. Med. Assoc., 310, pp. 2191, 10.1001/jama.2013.281053
  • Young, (2013), J. Child Neurol., 28, pp. 1238, 10.1177/0883073812460092
  • Zhang, (2005), Psychometrika, 70, pp. 203, 10.1007/s11336-003-1119-8